The war against standards in college education continues to feature unscientific discourse. Consider this PBS piece, which starts with:
Standardized test scores in the U.S. used to all but make or break college applications for high school seniors.
Nope. U.S. colleges have not - for as long as I've lived - determined admission based solely on standardized test scores. In fact, the admission policy of the typical competitive American college differs from those in many other countries, primarily because they purport to evaluate applicants "holistically," incorporating subjective criteria such as high school GPAs, extracurricular activites, and athletics.
The PBS host then went on to describe a student who scored 18 out of 36 on the ACT, was told by Google that she should apply to community colleges instead of Emory, and complained that "[...] didn't consider my grades as an A student". This has multiple fallacies wrapped into an incoherent mess. Why should Google stop any student from applying to Emory? If she did apply, the school would have looked at her high school GPA because of the "holistic" admission policy. In fact, most Emory applicants are likely to have scored above 18 on the ACT, and those who scored above 18 are likely to also be A students at their high schools.
Later PBS informed us that this particular student chose to ignore Google, Emory did use a holistic application policy, and she did get admitted. Instead of writing a complimentary piece about the value of holistic policies in dealing with certain "edge cases" (what Gladwell would have called outliers), this journalist decided to use such edge cases to launch an attack on standardized testing.
***
Nevertheless, during Covid, many colleges made standardized test scores optional in the application process; and many colleges have decided since to remove standardized testing from their "holistic" admission policy. Universities like Berkeley and Chicago stopped requiring SAT scores prior to Covid; other schools like Columbia, and William & Mary have boarded the ship.
According to the PBS article, the reason is that these tests are "biased." This is one of my favorite topics, which I discussed extensively in Chapter 3 of Numbers Rule Your World (link). Bias is a word that is thrown around casually, with little appreciation of what it means. In the PBS article, they used a single example to define it:
White students were doing better on more heavily weighted easier items with common words like golf and canoe, but Black and Latinx students did better on items that were more academic and more difficult, although those were weighted less heavily, so, words like vehemence and sycophant, which I can barely say.
This is a very specific instance of a very convoluted type of bias, exactly the kind that is well-researched by ETS, the entity that runs the SAT. In the book, I cover the statistical discipline called "differential item functioning," which are fascinating experiments embedded into standardized tests that measure precisely this sort of biases of individual test items, a first step toward correcting for them.
The justification of bias elimination is typically the pursuit of "objectivity" or "fairness." What do these test critics propose as replacement for standardized testing? Teacher evaluations is one alternative mentioned by the PBS host.
What about teacher evaluation is "objective"? I have read thousands of applicant reference letters - the vast majority are routine letters that tell me nothing ("Student X is one of my best students who worked diligently and earned an A in my class."). The memorable ones are always personal - personal references are the opposite of "objective" or "fair".
I have read some extremely negative personal references as well. The writers clearly had taken on the mission to warn admission officers about the applicants. I had chosen to ignore them in the past. Why? Because such personal references are too subjective - I knew neither the applicant nor the reference writer, I had been served a sample size of one, for every negative letter there is likely to be another reference who wrote a positive one, so who was I to believe?
Flipped to the other side, why should I believe extremely positive personal references? The same set of circumstances applies.
Does "holistic" help here? Surprisingly no. Of course, I can believe the extremely positive personal references if the student also participated in the Math Olympiad or got perfect SAT scores. But then, the references do not add much new information. The reference letters provide additional information if and only if they are uncorrelated with the rest of the application, i.e. the straight-A student with high test scores in which a reference might disclose as a cheater, or the student with low grades in which a reference might disclose as a victim of bullying. If it's uncorrelated with the rest of the portfolio, then this is just a question of trust. It's far, far from the ideal of "objectivity" that these test critics are demanding.
If eliminating "bias" were their chief concern, then the movement to abolish standardized testing from college admissions will fail. It merely substitutes one form of bias with another.
[Don't get me started on the bias due to the differential writing abilities of the teachers writing these reference letters. Apparently, these test critics are not worried about it as I haven't encountered the movement to ban reference letters yet. Worse, has there been a proper scientific study of biases inherent in personal recommendation letters? Have there been any attempts to correct such biases?]
***
I have to credit the PBS host for hinting at one of the little-known drivers of the anti-standardized testing movement. Many colleges tasted the forbidden fruit during the Covid pandemic when 80 percent of colleges decided to make test scores optional.
Because it became easier to submit applications, many colleges including Emory experienced a surge in the number of applicants. According to PBS, applications to Emory jumped by 20 percent in a year.
What the host didn't mention is what the application surge did to Emory's "selectivity" rate, which is the number of admission offers divided by the number of applicants. Since class size is fixed in the near term, any increase in applicants just lowers the selectivity rate. Quickly looking around the web, I learned that Emory's selectivity rate was 15.6% for Fall 2020. So what if we added 20% more applicants and left the number of offers the same. The new selectivity rate would be 15.6%/1.2 = 13%. And what did Google say Emory's selectivity rate was in Fall 2021? You guessed it: 13%. The entire increase in applicants translated to an apparent rise in selectivity.
Did the quality of the average applicant pool improve? If the incremental applicants consist of those who do not want to take the ACT, or those who have taken the ACT but elected to not disclose the score, I'll let you take a guess.
I noticed and wrote about this more than 10 years ago - you can read all about this tactic and others used by colleges and grad schools in Numbersense (Chapter 1, link).
While the absolute selectivity rate of Emory has gone up, its relative selectivity rank against other competitive colleges might not have. That's because one of the enablers of the applicant surge is the Common App, a brilliant invention if there was ever.
This App, purportedly a time saver for college applicants, is a tide that raises all boats. The Common App increases the total number of applications filed across the entire pool of colleges that utilize it. It's the same concept as Amazon's one-click purchase. All these schools will experience a surge in applications and a corresponding improvement in selectivity, as Emory saw.
Of course, the Common App has been in play for a while. The new battleground is standardized test scores. Removing this requirement lowers the barrier to filing an application. Those schools that made it test optional are now riding the next tide. What will college admissions offices do after tasting the forbidden fruit?
***
One more thing. The PBS host was angry that Google had recommended that the student not apply to Emory due to an ACT score well below the average ACT score of prior admitted students. Ironically, one of the benefits of standardized test scores - and generally, of any statistical metrics - is its signaling value.
Using test scores, students and parents are able to make realistic assessments of their chances of admission at selective colleges, and opt out of applying, saving both time and money. Now, students can apply blissfully, and enter a lottery with very low odds of winning. The chance of admission for someone who scored 18 out of 36 on ACT to Emory is nearly zero. Without these scores, the schools pretend that everyone has a chance.
To learn a lot more tricks used by admission officers, see Chapter 1 of Numbersense (link).
Recent Comments